September 14, 2020 | 12:23

How People Learn — Memory Storage Types

Memory Storage Types Cover

Human memory is captivating. Take a moment to dive into your oldest recollections and evoke any memory related to the following:

  • the first memory you can recall;
  • your first friend;
  • your first pet;
  • your childhood toys;

As mentioned in How People Learn — The Brain Basics, memories are well established neural patterns that get activated simultaneously. These circuits encode our memories, abilities, behaviors, knowledge, literally everything.

Interestingly, though, there are different types of memory storage, and in this article, I’d like to describe what those are.

From High-Resolution, Short-Lived To Low-Resolution, Eternal Timespan Memory

Nature did a fascinating job in designing life (and us). We don’t even think about how we form memories, and, incidentally, we don’t yet fully understand this process. What we do know is that there are several stages the information from the outer world has to pass through until it becomes part of our long-lasting memories.

  • Sensory Memory
  • Short-Term Memory
  • Working Memory
  • Long-Term Memory

A note of caution before we proceed: human memory is an active area of research that still has many unknowns, and there are many models and theories about some aspects of it. As we do not yet fully understand the internal workings of the brain, scientists have to rely on some simplifications and approximation models to explain how we receive, process, and perceive information. If you have heard about a different model or disagree with the models presented here, I kindly invite you to discuss it in the comments!

Sensory Memory

Sensory memory is the first place where sensory information gets deposited for further use. Data from human senses is stored here in a relatively unprocessed form for a brief period.

Sensory memory consists of distinct storage types for three of human senses:

  • Iconic Memory is for the exclusive storage of visual information;
  • Echoic Memory is for the exclusive storage of auditory information;
  • Haptic Memory is for the exclusive storage of tactile stimuli;

It is considered that Sensory memory is outside of cognitive control. It is the first step in the perception of the environment around us. As mentioned in how we see a pixelated version of the world, information quality degrades as it moves from our senses to the central executive system. Sensory memory is the first frontier toward ingesting and making sense of data, and at this point, the information is of very high quality.

However, storing such detailed information comes at a price. It is incredibly volatile, as it fades away quickly. The availability differs between senses, though:

  • Iconic memory’s availability is <1 second.1 That’s why you don’t remember what time it is, even though you just looked at your watch!
  • Echoic memory’s availability is somewhere between 2–4 seconds.2 That’s why, whenever you don’t pay attention when someone’s talking, and they ask you to repeat what they just said, you still can access those several seconds worth of information from your Echoic memory store.
  • Haptic memory’s availability is <2 seconds.3

Short-Term Memory (STM)

STM is a very ambiguous term that supposedly originated at the beginning of the twentieth century. It is often used interchangeably with Working Memory, which I will also use here. Other usages relate to simply holding the information (in contrast to storing and processing it, as Working Memory does) over a short period. But as you will see below, one Working Memory model encompasses some components that fit the description of an STM store. Therefore, whenever you see in the literature STM, it most probably is the Working Memory, which is a more recent term that better fits the description of cognitive processes related to information processing and storage.

Working Memory (WM)

Once the information from the outer world reaches our senses and is stored in the sensory memory, some of it requires conscious processing and manipulation, and the Working Memory is the place where that happens. The nature of working memory is still under active debate, as many models have been proposed to explain its essence. However, only two of them are the most influential:

  • The multicomponent model by Baddeley and Hitch;
  • Working memory as part of long-term memory by Ericsson and Kintsch;

The Multicomponent Model

Baddeley and Hitch multicomponent model

Source: Wikipedia

According to Baddeley, the WM consists of several components. The Central Executive component coordinates attention and cognitive processes, suppresses irrelevant information and actions, and switches between various tasks. It also is responsible for coordinating the subordinate systems (Phonological Loop and Visuo-Spatial Scratchpad).

The Phonological Loop (PL) stores phonological information (sound and language). Interestingly, visually presented language (like the text you are reading here) can be transformed into phonological information by subvocalization. PL consists of an Acoustic Store (which partially represents the Sensory Memory for auditory information) and an Articulatory Loop that continuously refreshes data in the Acoustic Store in a rehearsal loop.

The Visuo-Spatial Sketchpad (VSS) is a similar structure to PL but for visual and spatial information. It connects with the visual sensory memory store but, at the same time, can be used to construct and manipulate images and represent mental maps. If I ask you to imagine an oak tree on a green field on a sunny day, you will engage the VSS to perform that action.

The original model was proposed in 1974 and consisted only of these three components, but in 2000, Baddeley extended the model by adding a fourth one, the Episodic Buffer (EP). Its addition was to address some concerns regarding the flexibility of the model. The EP is supposed to hold representations that integrate phonological, visual, and spatial information and information not covered by the subordinate systems (e.g., semantic, musical, categorical information). It is episodic because it is assumed to bind information into a unitary episodic representation.

This model does a good job portraying the inner processes of the Working Memory. However, it is not exhaustive. For example, it does not provide a representation for tactile sensory information.4

Working memory as part of long-term memory

The central executive of working memory is retrieving memory from long-term memory.

Source: Wikipedia

Contrary to the multicomponent modal, Anders Ericsson and Walter Kintsch have introduced the notion of “long-term working memory,” which they define as a set of “retrieval structures” in long-term memory that enables seamless access to the information relevant for everyday tasks.5

According to this model, the Working Memory is not a separate system but rather a mechanism to activate and operate on parts of the long-term memory. The activated long-term memory representations can then be brought to our awareness through the focus of attention.

In terms of capacity, since activated working memory representations rely on the existing long-term memory representations, there can be many of these. But the bottleneck is the focus of attention, which is very narrow and can hold up to four such activated representations at once. I will discuss this in more detail in a follow-up post.

Long-Term Memory (LTM)

Finally, LTM is the memory storage that holds all our memories, old and new, implicit, and explicit. The most common analogy is that the LTM is like a hard-disk: you write information to it, and it is stored there. It is the warehouse of all the things we have read, heard, saw, and experienced.

The Whole Brain

As mentioned earlier, there are still many unknowns, both at the level of each store type as well as at their joints. But the information we know already gives us a possibility to improve our productivity and the way we learn. In the next articles, I will dive deeper into some of these stores and show you what causes poor performance and what to do about it.

If you liked this article, consider subscribing below and following me on twitter (@iuliangulea).

  1. The information available in brief visual presentations. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 74, 1-29 — George Sperling ↩︎

  2. An auditory analogue of the sperling partial report procedure: Evidence for brief auditory storage — Christopher J.Darwin, Michael T.Turvey, Robert G.Crowder ↩︎

  3. Dubrowski, Carnahan, Shih (2009), “Evidence for Haptic Memory," Third Joint EuroHaptics conference and Symposium on Haptic Interfaces for Virtual Environment and Teleoperator Systems, pp. 145–149 ↩︎

  4. Nelson Cowan raises this concern in his book “Working Memory Capacity” ↩︎

  5. Working memory as part of long-term memory — Wikipedia ↩︎


Subscribe to receive more posts like this

* indicates required